forked from twu-fhss/psyc106
-
Notifications
You must be signed in to change notification settings - Fork 0
/
u7.qmd
227 lines (128 loc) · 20.3 KB
/
u7.qmd
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
# Social Psychology - Part 2
## Overview {.unnumbered}
In this unit, we continue to build upon Unit 6. No single factor has more impact on shaping the quality and direction of your life over virtually every aspect of your existence than relationships. In a way, everyone is a social psychologist. People constantly form ideas/opinions about why a person or a group of people are acting the way they are. Social psychology is similar to what people find themselves naturally doing except it uses scientific tools to study these hypotheses (the ideas/opinions). Social psychologist are primarily interested in examining the different ways that individual’s effect groups of people and the way groups of people effect individual’s. In this unit you will be introduced to some historical studies and main concepts in this field. Topics covered in this chapter include social influences on behaviour, social impressions and judgements, the relationship between attitudes and behaviour, and effective communication. In Unit 6 (Part 1) you looked at attributes and actions, conformity, obedience, and compliance.
Please note that this unit will also examine prejudice and aggression. Although these topics are not specifically covered in the textbook, they are important to social psychology, attraction, and altruism.
### Topics {.unnumbered}
This unit is divided into the following topics:
1. Prejudice and Aggression
2. Attraction and Altruism
### Learning Outcomes {.unnumbered}
By the end of this unit, student's will be able to:
- Define the key terminology associated with social influence, social cognition, and on attitudes, behaviour, and effective communication.
- Describe why individuals conform to others’ behaviours, how individuals and groups can influence behaviours, and how we form first impressions and how these impressions influence us.
- Apply your knowledge of the bystander effect to ensure that you will be helped if you are in an emergency, of social cognition to help overcome prejudice and discrimination, and of the central route to describe how a message should be designed.
- Analyze whether guards who participate in abuse are inherently bad people, or if their behaviour is the product of social influences and whether people who commit discriminatory acts are necessarily prejudiced.
- Describe how behaviours influence attitudes in terms of cognitive dissonance theory.
### Learning Activities
Here is a checklist of learning activities you will benefit from in completing this unit. You may find it useful for planning your work.
- Read the relevant sections of Chapter 13 of your textbook
- Review the Chapter 13 - Notes (intended to support your understanding of your readings)
- Read about *Discussing Hate and Violence with Children,* *Resolution on Violent Video Games,* and *Television and Video Violence*
- Read and Reflect on the articles *Facial Attractiveness,* *Survival of the Prettiest,* *Altruism in Advertising,* and *Random Acts of Kindess*
- Complete the Key Terms quiz (ungraded)
:::
::: {.note icon="true"}
***Note***
The course units follow topics in the textbook, *Revel for An Introduction to Psychological Science* by Krause et al. (4th Edition). For each unit, please read the pertinent chapter(s) before completing the assessment for the unit.
:::
::: {.note}
***Assessment***
In this course you demonstrate your understanding of the course learning outcomes in different ways, including papers, projects, discussions and quizzes. Please see the Assessment section in Moodle for assignment details and due dates.
:::
### Resources {.unnumbered}
Here are the resources you will need to complete this unit:
- Krause, M., Corts, D., & Smith, S. C. (2024). *Revel for An Introduction to Psychological Science, 4th Canadian Edition.* Pearson Ed.
- Other resources will be provided online.
## Prejudice and Aggression
### Prejudice & Religion {.unnumbered}
Some of the first research to study the effects of religion (Christianity) investigated its role in prejudice. The consistent finding of this line of study was that the more religious people were, the more prejudiced they were likely to be Allport and Kramer (1946). Because racial prejudice is so clearly contrary to religious teaching, Gordon Allport called this Religion's Grand Paradox. In the area of racial prejudice in North America, Christianity did not appear to improve behavior.
Religion's Grand Paradox was gradually resolved as researchers refined their definition of religious. When the apparently prejudiced church attenders were divided into those for whom religion served a social function and was a means to an end (extrinsic religious orientation) and those for whom religion was an integral part of their lives and an end in itself (intrinsic religious orientation), it was the extrinsics who had raised the prejudice scores of the entire group. The intrinsics were found to be even less prejudiced than the non-attenders. Apparently an intrinsic religious orientation has a beneficial effect on behavior, at least on racial prejudice (Allport, 1959).
### Sports as Catharsis? {.unnumbered}
The idea that relieving emotional tension causes a purging of pent-up energy is called the catharsis hypothesis. According to Aristotle—who first proposed the concept of catharsis—even viewing powerful emotional events can be cathartic. Thus we should be able to reduce our aggressive tendencies by watching others act out their aggression, as well as by acting aggressively ourselves.
Research, however, does not lend much support to this common-sense idea. For example, Robert Arms and his associates report that hockey, football, and wrestling fans exhibit more aggression after viewing the event than before (Arms et al., 1979; Goldstein & Arms, 1971; Russell, 1983) (see Figure 11.1). Arthur Patterson (1974) found that high school football players were more aggressive after the season than before it. And Dane Archer and Rosemary Gartner report that a nation's murder rate tends to jump after a war is over (Archer & Gartner, 1976).
### Compensatory Machoism {.unnumbered}
Marshall Segall and his colleagues point out that cultures typically expect men to be macho —that is, to behave in ways that indicate their strength, virility, and willingness to confront an adversary. But Segall and his colleagues suggest that biological mechanisms interact with complex cultural processes to produce a kind of "compensatory machoism." Very briefly, the elements in this process are these:
1. All societies have some division of labor by sex, resulting in sex-linked roles. For example, females do most of the child rearing in most societies and virtually all of it in some. Boys have less opportunity than girls to learn their roles by modeling because they are raised by women and have comparatively little contact with adult males.
2. For various historical and environmental reasons, fathers in some societies are more likely to be absent. Paradoxically, in these societies young males are expected to develop a particularly distinct male identity. These young males are driven—consciously or unconsciously—to display "manly" behavior and avoid appearing "womanly" at all costs.
3. Some societies recognize this need and provide initiation ceremonies that serve to "stamp in" masculinity for boys who need it because of inadequate opportunity to acquire it in childhood. These ceremonies affirm the young male's adult masculinity.
4. Adolescent males in societies without initiation ceremonies may exaggerate their biological tendencies toward aggression in an attempt to assert their own masculinity.
Segall and his associates explain, "We suggest that in societies that have the preconditions requiring a stamping-in of masculinity, but which don't achieve this through initiation ceremonies or other institutionalized practices, adolescent males will try on their own to assert their masculinity" (p. 281). The Segall group calls the resultant aggression compensatory machoism. Compensatory machoism is the display of behavior intended to identify oneself with the cultural image of masculinity. The resulting aggression is rooted not in anger but in a "felt need to escape from womanliness and to mark one's masculine gender" (p. 281).
::: {.note icon="true"}
***Can you think of a cultural group in North America that seems to illustrate this process particularly well?***
:::
Marshall Segall and his colleagues suggest that many young black American males illustrate this process. Social conditions (unemployment, educational deficits, racial discrimination) contribute to a prevalence of broken homes with female-headed households. Young men in these conditions attempt to assert their masculine identity through compensatory machoism. They are tough, independent, and violently aggressive. As a result, murder is their leading cause of death. A black man in stands a 1-in-21 chance of being murdered in his lifetime, compared with 1 in 131 for white men and 1 in 369 for white women. The Segall group quotes one young black male who says that in urban ghettos *"You must walk angry, talk angry, think angry . . . . You're either a chump or a champ . . . . You're macho or gay." (adapted from Invitation to Social Psychology by Ronald Philipchalk)*
### Empathy & Aggression {.unnumbered}
Several researchers suggest that empathy and concern for others may be important tools for reducing aggression. Prejudice, discrimination, and violent sexual aggression flourish when we dehumanize a victim. Empathy reverses this process by making us identify with a potential victim. For example, Ken-Ichi Ohbuchi and his colleagues found that Japanese students gave less shock to a "victim" if the victim first aroused subjects' empathy by disclosing information about herself or by expressing her fear of shock. *(adapted from Invitation to Social Psychology by Ronald Philipchalk)*
### Conflict Resolution & Culture {.unnumbered}
Fumie Kumagai and Murray Straus report significant differences in the way conflicts are resolved between marital partners in different cultures. Using the "Conflict Tactics Scale," these researchers asked high school seniors in, and the United States how their parents settled disputes. Violent means of "resolution" were most common in American couples, followed by Japanese, and then Indian couples.
<!-- [plugin:content-inject](_activities){target="_blank"} -->
### Activity: Read and Reflect
::: {.learning-activity}
The following articles are intended to deepen your understanding of discrimination, racism, hate and violence. Hopefully this information will cause you to consider how you’ve been informed about these topics. Additionally, it may be valuable, in light of the research, to consider how much hate and violence you have exposure to through gaming and TV and how this is impacting your attitude and behaviour towards other people. Click on the articles below:
- [**Discussing Hate and Violence with Children**](https://www.adl.org/education/resources/tools-and-strategies/discussing-hate-and-violence-with-children){target="_blank"}
- [**Resolution on Violent Video Games**](https://www.apa.org/about/policy/violent-video-games){target="_blank"}
- [**Television and Video Violence**](https://www.apa.org/action/resources/research-in-action/protect){target="_blank"}
:::
### Activity: Questions for Consideration
::: {.learning-activity}
Take a moment to reflect on what you have learned in this section. Use the following questions to guide your reflection:
- ***If removing parts of the brain reliably reduced aggression, do you think violent criminals should be forced to have this operation?***
- ***Should violent content be removed from video games, movies, and TV (why or why not)?***
*Be prepared to share your thoughts and insights with other members of the class*
:::
## Attraction & Altruism
*Why do I like you? Let me count the ways:*
- We often like people for whom we have suffered.
- We like people more after we have done small favours for them.
- We like people best whose (accidentally overheard) descriptions of us begin on a negative note and end with a positive tone.
- We like people who convert to our point of view better than we like people who have agreed with us all along.
- We like highly competent people better after they have committed a blunder.
### Culture & Attraction {.unnumbered}
The characteristics that are attractive in the opposite sex, as well as the importance of physical attractiveness, are different in different cultures. Deborah Stiles and her colleagues found that Mexican adolescents valued liking children, helping others, high intelligence, and inner goodness and honesty in their opposite-sex ideal. American adolescents gave higher ratings to having a lot of money, being fun, popular, good looking, and sexy. Karen Dion and her colleagues found that physical attractiveness is more important in a culture where individualism (Canadian) rather than collectivism (Chinese) is valued.
David Buss and 49 collaborators in 33 countries asked young men and women (average age 23 years) about the attributes they looked for in potential mates. The researchers found many differences among cultures. The most pervasive difference they found was the emphasis given to "traditional" versus "modern" attributes.
American young people differed from the international average by placing a higher value on a college degree, earning capacity, heredity, and "an exciting personality." They placed a relatively low value on housekeeping skills, and intelligence (Buss, 1990). Without this kind of information it would be easy for us to assume that people in other cultures look for the same qualities in their potential mates as we do. *(adapted from Understanding Human Behavior by Ronald Philipchalk)*
### Culture & Altruism {.unnumbered}
Benjamin Colby reports that differences in altruism between students from 133 different cultures were predictive of their ability to adapt to university culture (altruistic students adapted better). In one study Wegner and Crano found that racial factors affected helping behavior. An experimenter who was apparently engrossed in examining a deck of 500 computer cards approached naive subjects. Approximately one step away from the subject, the experimenter "accidentally" dropped the entire deck. African Americans helped Caucasians 17 percent of the time but they helped African Americans 75 percent of the time. Caucasians helped both Caucasians and African Americans 44 percent of the time.
::: {.note icon="true"}
***Note:***
*Cross-cultural research seems particularly relevant to the topic of helping in that Jesus point in the parable of the "Good Samaritan" was that cultural enemies, Jews and Samaritans, should love and help each other*
:::
### Altruism & Religion {.unnumbered}
The study of altruism or helping behavior also yields some interesting results with regard to the effects of religion. Research has consistently found that only a minority of people will stop to help someone who is clearly in need, especially if they are in a hurry themselves. Darley and Batson (1973) found that this was true even for seminary students, and *even when they were on their way to give a talk on the Good Samaritan.* Furthermore, they found that the most "religiously devout" were the least sensitive to the victims needs; when they did offer help, they often forced it on the victim even when he or she insisted on being left alone. Batson and Gray (1981) also found that the religiously devout tended to offer help when it was not wanted. Apparently if religion makes a difference it is not always good.
On the other hand, it should be noted that although only a minority of the seminarians stopped to help, they were *all* on their way to help the experimenter by giving a talk. Thus it was not a matter of helping or not, but a choice between which to help. If they stopped, they were led to believe, they would be late, and might endanger their ability to be of help to the experimenter.
In another setting, Perry London (1970) found Christian teaching often played a role in the justification given by those who helped hide Jews from the Nazis during World War II. Further research is needed before the role of religiousness in helping behavior can be determined. *(adapted from Psychology and Christianity by Ronald Philipchalk, pp. 219-220)*
<!-- [plugin:content-inject](_activities){target="_blank"} -->
### Activity: Read and Reflect
::: {.learning-activity}
What is it that makes you think that someone is attractive, kind, evil, competent, upset, and the like? These resources will look at some of the factors that contribute to how you judge others and yourself. The first article outlines some of the ways your face has shaped your social experiences. The second article adds to this discussion by looking at how willing people are to acknowledge the role of attractiveness in their own thought processes and decision-making. Next, the topic shifts to altruism. The first of these articles proposes that only reciprocal altruism exists. Again, I would encourage you to read this article through the lens of, *“Is it truly possible to know another’s motivation or intention?”* Lastly, Random Acts of Kindness is a website that offers great resources and opportunities to increase kindness in your, and others, lives. I encourage you to participate in some of these activities to see how it makes you feel. Click on the links below:
- [**Facial Attractiveness**](https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3130383/){target="_blank"}
- [**Survival of the Prettiest**](https://www.macleans.ca/society/science/the-mysterious-power-of-attractive-people/){target="_blank"}
- [**Altruism in Advertising**](https://public.wsu.edu/~taflinge/altruism.html){target="_blank"}
- [**Random Acts of Kindess**](http://www.actsofkindness.org/){target="_blank"}
:::
### Activity: Chapter 13 Key Terms Quiz
::: {.learning-activity}
In order to review some of the major terms from Chapter 13 in your textbook, take the following unmarked quiz. Although you will not be evaluated on these terms, they will assist you in the assessments for this course. Type the correct answer in the boxes below:
<iframe src="https://create.twu.ca/h5p/wp-admin/admin-ajax.php?action=h5p_embed&id=89" width="936" height="794" frameborder="0" allowfullscreen="allowfullscreen" title="Psych 106 - Unit 7 Terminology"></iframe><script src="https://create.twu.ca/h5p/wp-content/plugins/h5p/h5p-php-library/js/h5p-resizer.js" charset="UTF-8"></script>
:::
### Activity: Questions for Consideration
::: {.learning-activity}
Read through the following questions and reflect on what you have learned:
- ***What do you think contribute to you being able to quickly judge whether or not you think someone is attractive?***
- ***Some social psychologists believe that there is always a reward for helping and that this motivates people to help. In other words, people never help without expecting a reward of some kind (either in this life or in the next). What do you think?***
*Be prepared to share your thoughts and insights with other members of the class*
:::
## Assessment {.unnumbered}
::: {.note}
Refer to the course schedule for graded assignments you are responsible for submitting. **All graded assignments, and their due dates, can be found on the "Assessment" tab.**
In addition to any graded assignments you are responsible for submitting, be sure to complete all the Learning Activities that have been provided throughout the content - these are intended to support your understanding of the content.
:::
## Checking Your Learning {.unnumbered}
::: {.check}
Before you move on to the next unit, check that you are able to:
- Define the key terminology associated with social influence, social cognition, and on attitudes, behaviour, and effective communication.
- Describe why individuals conform to others’ behaviours, how individuals and groups can influence behaviours, and how we form first impressions and how these impressions influence us.
- Apply your knowledge of the bystander effect to ensure that you will be helped if you are in an emergency, of social cognition to help overcome prejudice and discrimination, and of the central route to describe how a message should be designed.
- Analyze whether guards who participate in abuse are inherently bad people, or if their behaviour is the product of social influences and whether people who commit discriminatory acts are necessarily prejudiced.
- Describe how behaviours influence attitudes in terms of cognitive dissonance theory.
:::